Basic experimental studies involving humans (BESH) are studies that meet both the definition of basic research and the NIH definition of a clinical trial. BESH therefore are subject to NIH clinical trials policies such as registration and results reporting.
All BESH meet the NIH definition of a clinical trial, but not all clinical trials are BESH.
Key Characteristics of BESH
Basic Experimental Studies involving Humans (BESH) are studies that fall within the NIH definition of a clinical trial and also meet the definition of basic research. Here are some key characteristics to help identify if your study fits within a BESH funding opportunity:
The study has the purpose of understanding fundamental aspects of phenomena without specific application towards processes or products in mind
We define “process or products” as the application of biomedical or behavioral products, procedures, or services intended to affect a health-related outcome either by better understanding the mechanism of action of an intervention or a measurable improvement in health. If the study does have specific applications toward processes or products in mind (e.g., Phase I drug or device study), then the application should be submitted under a clinical trial funding opportunity, not a BESH funding opportunity.
Basic ResearchA systematic study directed toward greater knowledge or understanding of the fundamental aspects of phenomena and of observable facts without specific applications towards processes or products in mind. (32 CFR 272.3) |
Clinical TrialA research study in which one or more human subjects are prospectively assigned to one or more interventions (which may include placebo or other control) to evaluate the effects of those interventions on health-related biomedical or behavioral outcomes. |
The study uses an experimental manipulation/intervention to understand a basic phenomenon
Basic research uses a range of probes or experimental manipulations to perturb a physiological process (including cognitive and perceptual processes). Under the NIH definition of a clinical trial, most experimental manipulations involving humans are considered to be interventions.
The intervention/experimental manipulation is not intended to change the health status of the participants
This definition is not limited to manipulations that improve health-related endpoints in a long-term fashion. Many experimental manipulations are considered to be interventions even if the manipulation is being used to study the normal function of a physiological system and the effects are short-lived, reversible, and/or benign.
Answering the 4 Clinical Trial Questions for BESH
There are 4 key questions that determine if a study meets the NIH definition of a clinical trial. Here are some specific things to consider when answering these questions in the context of basic research studies.
Does the study involve human participants? | |
By definition, BESH involve human participants. According to 45 CFR 46, a human subject is a living individual about whom an investigator (whether professional or student) conducting research:
For further information about whether your study qualifies as human subjects research, see 45 CFR 46. |
|
Are the participants prospectively assigned to an intervention? | |
|
Prospective assignment is not limited to random assignment to a manipulation or condition. “Prospectively assigned” means the experimental manipulation (intervention) is assigned per a predefined protocol. The method of prospective assignment also does not matter. Neither does the number of conditions. For more about interventions, see question #3 below. |
Is the study designed to evaluate the effect of the intervention on the participants? | |
For BESH, the “intervention” is often an experimental manipulation being used as a probe to understand a basic phenomenon, process, or the mechanism of action of the manipulation. Interventions are manipulations of the subject or the subject’s environment that are performed for research purposes. For BESH, interventions include a wide range of probes, tasks, or other procedures to control, isolate, and/or manipulate a study variable (typically the independent variable). For example, evaluating the effect of the intervention on the participants in BESH often takes the form of observing the effect of the manipulation on a physiological, psychological, or social process (including cognitive and perceptual processes). It is useful for BESH investigators to think of an experimental intervention as an independent variable, which the investigator changes or controls. The intervention (ie, independent variable) will have a direct effect on a participant which can be measured (i.e., directly effects a dependent variable which is tested and measured in an experiment). In contrast to BESH, researchers conducting observational studies do not control an independent variable. They observe and measure variables of interest (in some cases, over time) and look for relationships between them. The variables are measured, but not manipulated. For help distinguishing an intervention versus a measurement, see below. |
|
Is the effect being evaluated a health-related biomedical or behavioral outcome? | |
Studies applying for NIH funding should have biomedical or behavioral outcomes in mind. In BESH, the outcomes are often biomedical or behavioral phenomenon or processes, or the mechanism of action of a biomedical or behavioral manipulation. These outcomes, even if transitory and benign, are considered health-related biomedical or behavioral outcomes. |
Intervention vs. Measurement
It is possible that the same specific task or test could represent either an “intervention” or a “measurement” depending on how it is used, what is being measured, and why. Because of this, there cannot be a list of tests that are always measurements, or always interventions.
If the task or test is being used to purposefully change or manipulate something about the participant and is being used to explore a concept – the task or test is probably an intervention. Think of the intervention as the independent variable in a study.
The two example cases below use the Stroop Effect, an established measure of executive function in which a person’s response time to name the color of words presented is measured. In the standard version of the task, the colors of words presented can be the same or different from the meaning of the word itself. The Stroop Effect is the slowing in color naming time for incongruent word presentations. Sometimes investigators modify the design, for example, to study the effect of emotional content of the words (i.e. the independent variable) on the Stroop Effect. As such, depending on how the Stroop task is employed, it may or may not be considered an intervention.
Example A
A group of adolescents will participate in a longitudinal study examining changes in executive function over the course of a normal school year. Color naming performance on the standard version of the Stroop test will be obtained. All measures will be compared at multiple time points during the school year to examine changes in executive function. The purpose is to observe changes in executive function and to observe if differences exist in the Stroop effect over the course of the school year for these adolescents.
- Does the study involve human participants? Yes, adolescents will be enrolled in this study.
- Are the participants prospectively assigned to an intervention? No, there is no intervention in this study and no independent variable manipulated. The adolescents are not prospectively assigned to an intervention, but instead the investigator will examine variables of interest (including the Stroop test) over time. The Stroop effect is used as a measurement of point-in-time data.
- Is the study designed to evaluate the effect of the intervention on the participants? N/A, there is no intervention. Performance on the Stroop test is a well-established measure of executive function and the test is not providing an independent variable of interest here. It is not being used to manipulate the participants or their environment. The purpose is simply to obtain a measure of executive function in adolescents over the course of the school year.
- Is the effect being evaluated a health-related biomedical or behavioral outcome? N/A. No effect of an intervention is being evaluated.
Example B
A group of participants with social anxiety will perform an experimentally manipulated Stroop test. In this variant of the Stroop test, the stimuli presented are varied to include emotional and neutral facial expressions presented in different colors. Participants are instructed to name the colors of the faces presented, with the expectation that they will be slower to name the color of the emotional face than the neutral face. The purpose of the study is to examine the degree to which participants with social anxiety will be slower to process emotional faces than neutral faces.
- Does the study involve human participants? Yes, participants with social anxiety will be enrolled in this study.
- Are the participants prospectively assigned to an intervention? Yes, the participants will be prospectively assigned to perform a modified Stroop test using different colored emotional/neutral faces to explore emotional processing in people with social anxiety. Note that the independent variable is the presentation of emotional vs neutral faces.
- Is the study designed to evaluate the effect of the intervention on the participants? Yes, the study is designed to measure the effect of emotional valence (i.e. emotional faces) on participant response time to name the color. The purpose is to determine whether the response time to emotional faces is exaggerated for people with social anxiety as compared to neutral faces. Note that the response time to name the colors is the dependent variable in this study.
- Is the effect being evaluated a health-related biomedical or behavioral outcome? Yes, the processing of emotional information is a health-related biomedical outcome.
As these examples show, the context of how a task is used is important in determining whether it is being used as an
intervention or a measurement.
About BESH Funding Opportunities
NIH has issued a series of parent funding opportunities specifically designated as "Basic
Experimental
Studies with
Humans Required" in the funding opportunity's title. Refer to our chart of funding opportunity types
by clinical
trial
allowability to determine what funding opportunity may fit you best.
Participation in BESH funding opportunities will vary by NIH Institute and Center (IC). Be sure to
check the
participating organizations section of the funding opportunity and talk with a Program Officer to
determine
the most
appropriate funding opportunity for your application.
Policy Flexibilities for BESH Funding Opportunities
BESH studies are subject to the NIH Policy on the Dissemination of NIH-Funded Clinical Trial Information. However, NIH recognizes that registering and reporting results in ClinicalTrials.gov may pose a challenge for some types of BESH. NIH has delayed the enforcement for ClinicalTrials.gov registration and results reporting through September 24, 2025.
This delayed enforcement is only applicable to BESH studies submitted to funding opportunities designated as “basic experimental studies with humans” in the title. It does not apply to applications submitted under a clinical trials required or clinical trials optional funding opportunity.
During this period of delayed enforcement, NIH continues to expect registration and results reporting for studies submitted to BESH funding opportunities, but with the additional flexibility to register and report results on alternative publicly available platforms.
Plans for meeting the NIH reporting expectations using an alternative platform should be described at the time of application in the Dissemination Plan attachment. Funded awardees for applications submitted to BESH-specific funding opportunities who are not using ClinicalTrials.gov to meet the policy expectation should provide in their annual progress reports the unique identifier assigned by the alternative platform, if available, and a link to the report (e.g., page or record) in the alternative platform.
NIH also continues to expect Good Clinical Practice (GCP) training for all personnel involved in the conduct, oversight, or management of Basic Experimental Studies involving Humans (BESH). Additionally, all such applications continue to require completion of the full PHS Human Subjects and Clinical Trials Information form, and will be evaluated using the clinical trial review criteria.
See NOT-OD-24-118 for additional details.
Tips for BESH Investigators
Talk to a Program Official: All applicants are strongly encouraged to talk with a Program Official before submitting an application. Program Officials can help determine the right funding opportunity for your specific research. If you are unsure who the right Program Official is, try out our Matchmaker Tool!
Review BESH Case Studies: Check out the BESH Case Studies to help understand what type of studies meet the criteria for a BESH funding opportunity.
Consider the Funding Opportunities: While BESH studies can be appropriately conducted under any “clinical trial required” or “clinical trial optional” funding opportunity, the policy flexibilities for BESH studies are only applicable to BESH studies conducted under specific BESH funding opportunities. Therefore, it is recommended that BESH studies apply to BESH funding opportunities when feasible.
Get to know the Human Subjects & Clinical Trial Information Form: Take a video tour of the application form to familiarize yourself with how this information is collected.