This Program Announcement expires three years from the Release Date shown
directly below.
BASIC AND TRANSLATIONAL RESEARCH IN EMOTION (R01)
Release Date: June 15, 2000 (see replacement PA-03-169)
PA NUMBER: PA-00-105
National Institute of Mental Health
National Institute on Aging
National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism
National Cancer Institute
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
National Institute on Drug Abuse
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
THIS PA USES "MODULAR GRANT" AND "JUST-IN-TIME" CONCEPTS. THIS PA INCLUDES
DETAILED MODIFICATIONS TO STANDARD APPLICATION INSTRUCTIONS THAT MUST BE USED
WHEN PREPARING AN APPLICATION IN RESPONSE TO THIS PA.
PURPOSE
This Program Announcement (PA) and PA-00-106 Basic And Translational Research
In Emotion: Small Grants (http://grants.nih.gov/grants/guide/pa-files/PA-00-106.html)
are revisions of PA-94-059, which was published in the NIH Guide on
April 29, 1994.
Under this PA, the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH), the National
Institute on Aging (NIA), the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism
(NIAAA), the National Cancer Institute (NCI), the National Institute of Child
Health and Human Development (NICHD), the National Institute on Drug Abuse
(NIDA), and the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS)
invite research grant applications to expand basic research on the processes and
mechanisms involved in the experience and expression of emotion.
HEALTHY PEOPLE 2010
The Public Health Service (PHS) is committed to achieving the health promotion
and disease prevention objectives of "Healthy People 2010," a PHS-led national
activity for setting priority areas. This Program Announcement (PA), Basic and
Translational Research in Emotion, is related to various priority areas.
Potential applicants may obtain a copy of "Healthy People 2010" at
http://www.health.gov/healthypeople.
ELIGIBILITY REQUIREMENTS
Applications may be submitted by domestic and foreign, for-profit and non-profit
organizations, public and private, such as universities, colleges, hospitals,
laboratories, units of State and local governments, and eligible agencies of the
Federal government. Racial/ethnic minority individuals, women, and persons with
disabilities are encouraged to apply as principal investigators.
MECHANISM OF SUPPORT
This PA will use the National Institutes of Health (NIH) R01 (research project
grant) mechanism. Responsibility for the planning, direction, and execution of
the proposed project will be solely that of the applicant. The total project
period for an application submitted in response to this PA may not exceed five
years.
For applications requesting up to $250,000 direct costs per year, funds must be
requested in $25,000 direct cost modules. A maximum of 10 modules per year may
be requested for R01 applications. The cost of equipment is included in the
budget limitation. Any necessary escalation must be included within the number
of modules being requested. Only limited budget information is required and any
budget adjustments made by the Initial Review Group will be in modules of
$25,000.
More detailed information about modular grant applications, including a sample
budget narrative justification pages and a sample biographical sketch, is
available via the Internet at:
http://grants.nih.gov/grants/funding/modular/modular.htm. Applications that
request more than $250,000 in any year must use the standard PHS 398 (rev. 4/98)
application instructions.
Applicants are strongly encouraged to consult with program staff (listed under
INQUIRIES).
RESEARCH OBJECTIVES
Background
The study of emotion encompasses a wide range of psychological, social,
developmental, and biological phenomena, including characteristic expressive
signals, alterations in physiology, and subjective feeling states. Central and
autonomic nervous system (CNS, ANS) activity and the role of cognitive
processing in emotion also are important objects of study, as is the
contribution of emotional and motivational systems to cognitive faculties such
as perception, attention, memory, and motor control. In addition, the study of
emotion includes overt behavioral responses such as aggression or withdrawal,
interpersonal relationships and communication, and environmental circumstances
and experiences that elicit and shape emotion. Emotion research can also
include the study of licit and illicit psychoactive substances that alter mood
states, and conversely, the study of how emotional and mood states can
predispose to, or modulate the effects of, pain or alcohol and psychoactive
substances. This PA also encourages research on emotional reactions in the
context of the diagnosis and treatment of cancer, and the study of emotion as it
relates to this disease or increased risk of this disease, including outcomes
such as social relationships, health care provider relationships, adherence and
others. Although most emotion research involves human populations, animal
models of emotion are also valuable for the investigation of particular aspects
of emotion and its eliciting contexts.
Recent years have shown the rapid expansion of concepts and methods for studying
emotion in all of its aspects. Outlined in this program announcement are
current needs that stem from these advances and that constitute critical
components of a comprehensive basic research strategy, with the ultimate aim of
fostering mental and physical health and the understanding of human development
and aging. Sample research questions are provided for illustration, they are
not intended to be exhaustive.
Basic Mechanisms of Emotion
The study of emotion involves measurements in a number of different response
systems (e.g., neural, physiological, subjective, expressive). To foster the
rapid and orderly accumulation of knowledge, it is important that multiple
system measurements be conducted whenever possible. Interactions of emotion
with cognition also constitute an important area of study. For example, more
detailed understanding is needed of the interplay between emotion and cognition
that can inform conceptualizations of disorders in which impairments of both
emotion and cognition are apparent (e.g., schizophrenia, depression, alcohol and
drug dependence, Parkinson’s Disease, Alzheimer’s Disease, autism), as well as
provide data important for promoting emotional self-regulation and understanding
developmental processes. In addition, the interplay between emotion and
cognition may be studied in the context of risk perception and decision-making
as it applies to treatment and screening among cancer patients or individuals at
increased risk of cancer, and in the context of financial and medical decision-
making by older adults. Sample research questions include the following:
o What are the relationships among behavioral, expressive, physiological, and
subjective aspects of emotion? What are the circumstances under which these
various systems act in concert, and what is the significance of various patterns
of desynchrony? What is the timing among the components and the developmental
course? What are the biological or psychological consequences of the inhibition
of one or more components?
o A number of different emotion theories posit some number of discrete
emotions. Other theories approach the domain as two, three or more dimensions
of emotional response. How are these two approaches related, and can they be
reconciled in a comprehensive theory of emotion?
o What are the continuities across, and distinctions among, the phenomena of
reflex, emotion, mood, temperament, emotional trait, and emotional disorder?
What social, psychological, and biological factors mediate their
interrelationships? How do these interrelationships change with age? How do
these phenomena interact in order to contribute to psychological adjustment,
normal psychological and biological development, treatment and screening
adherence, and quality of life among individuals with physical illness or those
at risk of illness?
o What are potential mechanisms by which sensation and perception influence
emotion? In turn, how are interactions between perception and emotion modulated
in the experience of pain, in learning and memory, and in cognitive development?
o How do attention and memory act to sustain or interrupt emotional states? In
turn, how do emotional states serve to modulate or drive mechanisms of attention
and memory? How are interactions between attention, memory, and emotion
modulated or modified by alcohol and psychoactive drugs? How do these
interactions change over the course of development?
Emotional Processes in Mental, Substance Abuse, Developmental, and Neurological
Disorders, and in Physical Disease
The study of emotional processes in disorders involves both similarities and
differences from research on basic mechanisms of emotion. Impairments of
emotion found in psychopathology and developmental disorders may differ in
either qualitative or quantitative ways from normal emotional processes.
Emotional reactions may interact with the course of disease processes to alter
the course of disorder. Examples of relevant issues include the following:
o What are the continuities and discontinuities between normative emotional
processes (e.g., emotional development, expression, understanding, awareness,
communication) and emotional processes seen in psychopathology, developmental
disorders, alcohol or drug abuse, or resilience?
o To what extent can behavioral, expressive, physiological, and subjective
measures of emotion identify intent in individuals at risk for suicidal,
violent, or self-injurious behavior within the context of preventive
interventions?
o Do individual differences in emotional reactivity and regulation, including
responses to stress, produce differential vulnerabilities to mental or
developmental disorders, including alcohol and/or drug dependence? Conversely,
does alcohol/drug use or dependence produce changes in emotional reactivity?
How do stimuli associated with alcohol or drug use become triggers of emotional
and subjective states that may lead to relapse?
o Among cancer patients and people at increased risk for cancer, how do
individual differences in emotional processes relate to reports of fatigue,
return to work, adherence to treatment, nutritional recommendations, cancer
screening behaviors, health care provider patient relationships, and other
sequelae of the cancer diagnosis or known risk status? What are the
psychological and/or health consequences of the use of denial/repression among
cancer patients?
o How can individuals be trained to best identify emotions and mood states that
may represent a possible risk for relapse of physical or mental illness? Does
focusing on such emotions/moods help prevent relapse, or actually increase risk
for relapse?
o How do cognitive changes associated with late aging modify the expression of
emotion and its underlying processes in older individuals?
o In neurologically impaired patients, are negative emotions, such as fear,
more prevalent? Are anxiety states mediated by the fear system? Are there
common neurobiological mechanisms for all kinds of fears such as social fear,
imagined fears, or existential fears?
Individual Differences
Research is suggesting that individual differences in emotional responsivity may
mark specific vulnerabilities to mental disorder, including alcohol or drug
dependence, the detailed examination of these individual differences is critical
for understanding etiology and for designing prevention efforts. In-depth study
is needed of the determinants, consequences, and sequelae of infant temperament.
Research in adult personality variation also is beginning to examine individual
differences in emotional responsivity, with some indications of connections to
physiology. Sample research questions include the following:
o What are the biological (including genetic) and experiential sources of
individual differences in emotional reactivity and regulation throughout
development? How do biological and experiential influences combine and
interact? How do these change with age?
o How do individual differences in emotionality relate to phenomena such as
activity level, attention, and cognitive processes? What are the neural
substrates that underlie relationships among such phenomena and how do these
relationships maintain or change over time?
o Among children with developmental and learning disabilities or childhood
illness, how are individual differences in emotional processes related to
functioning over time? How does the presence of developmental and/or learning
disabilities or other pediatric problems influence the development of emotional
regulation and reactivity over time?
o What biological, social, personality, and cognitive factors interact with
emotion-based individual differences to contribute to psychopathology and drug
use?
o How do emotions get attached to attitudes, stereotypes, and identity? How do
these influence illness and recovery?
Developmental Aspects
Data are accumulating rapidly in areas such as children"s understanding and
experience of emotions, and in emotional communications occurring between
parents and children beginning in the earliest weeks of life. The import of
findings related to the development of emotions would be well served by an
overarching theoretical framework specifying the ontogeny of emotion. Also, the
primary concentration to date on the early years of life needs to be broadened
to include focused attention on early and middle childhood, adolescence,
adulthood, and old age. Sample research questions include the following:
o Are connections among various components of emotional response present at
birth? Do they change with age, particularly during periods of transition
(e.g., adolescence)? How do changes in bodily systems with age affect the
nature and intensity of emotional response and the interrelationships among
response systems? Do such changes predispose toward psychopathology in older
individuals?
o What are the determinants, age-specific characteristics, and consequences of
emotional attachments across the lifespan? What are the parallels among
attachment patterns in infancy, in childhood, in adolescence, in adulthood, in
old age?
o How do cognitive factors (e.g., intelligence, learning disabilities)
influence the development of emotional processes over the lifespan?
o What affective processes are particularly germane to coping with events in
the family life cycle (e.g., marriage, divorce, birth, transition to parenthood,
aging, retirement, grandparenting, dealing with death and bereavement, coping
with substance use of family members)?
o What are the developmental psychobiological contributions of stress and other
environmental influences on emotional development and expression?
o What is the role of emotion in brain development over the life course and
what are the relevant mechanisms? What is the role of endocrine and immune
systems and their neural control over the course of development and aging?
o Does prenatal exposure to alcohol or abused drugs or use of abused drugs in
adolescence affect emotional development? Conversely, are children with
disorders of emotional regulation more vulnerable to becoming drug dependent?
Social Aspects
The quality of interpersonal relationships can be a significant source of both
positive and negative emotions. Further, social relationships play a
substantial role in the modulation of emotional responses, however generated.
Social factors thus make a critical contribution to an understanding of the
risks for mental disorders and alcohol/drug dependence. In addition to the need
for further research on these interpersonal aspects of emotion, it is very
important to examine the macro-environmental processes (e.g., culture, social
structure, the media) that help to shape emotional development and adjustment.
Sample research questions include the following:
o How do cultural and socialization processes influence the experience and
expression of emotion? How do salient social factors and contexts (e.g.,
childcare settings, media, exposure to violence) in particular developmental
stages shape affective development and expression?
o What are the dynamics of emotional communications occurring within families
and other intimate groups and how do they relate to the development,
maintenance, or erosion of emotional bonds? How do variations in social sharing
of emotions lead to differences in psychopathology, therapeutic approaches, and
potential health outcomes? How are these patterns altered by drug dependence?
Among cancer patients and significant others, what are the short- and long-term
consequences of patterns of emotional communication on social relationships and
psychological adjustment?
o How do variations in parenting style and behaviors (e.g., teaching, limit-
setting) influence the development of affect regulation in children? How does
child neglect (resulting from psychopathology, alcohol or drug addiction or
other causes) influence emotional development in children?
o How does caregiver behavior influence affect regulation in persons with
mental disorders in late life, including Alzheimer’s Disease?
o How do the emotions involved with social relationships affect life in the
community for severely disordered individuals, and how do these emotions
interact with the characteristics of the disorder to affect its course?
o What role does the process of social comparison play in the emotional
response to cancer and to aging, what are some potential mechanisms explaining
the direction of the social comparison process across individuals, and how might
these processes be influenced by other social or non-social mechanisms?
Biological Aspects
The study of emotion provides a valuable opportunity for examination of the
interplay between psychological, physiological, and neural processes, and
methods are increasingly becoming available for examining the neural substrates
of emotion. Sample research questions include the following:
o What are the bi-directional influences between emotional states or emotional
traits (e.g., temperament) and neurobiological, endocrine and immune systems?
Among cancer patients, how might these influences on biology influence health
status or treatment (side effects, ability to tolerate treatment)?
o What are the neuroanatomical circuits and neurochemical processes involved in
emotional states and emotion-based individual differences? How do these systems
evolve over the lifespan? To what extent do these neural processes overlap with
those associated with psychopathology and substance use disorders such as
alcohol or drug addiction?
o How can neuroimaging and large-array electrophysiological techniques best be
used to study brain areas that are active during different emotional states and
in pain perception? What is the relationship of observed CNS activity to other
responses in emotion, including alcohol/drug-induced or alcohol/drug withdrawal-
or craving-induced changes in emotion?
o How does the aging process, either in association with age-associated
neurogenerative disease processes or without disease, affect the emotion-based
networks within the brain and those that interact with the endocrine system?
Methodological Needs
Methods related to the study of emotion run the full range, from self-report and
interview procedures, to behavioral observations, and to measures of ANS and CNS
structure and function. Improvements are needed in ways that enhance validity
and efficiency of measurement without sacrificing richness and detail. In
research on physical illness such as cancer, also needed is methodology that
takes into account the reports of others in the patient’s social and medical
environment, and reports that are sensitive to the changes in emotional
responsiveness over time. Sample needs include the following:
o Most research on emotional expression concentrates on the face. Methods also
are needed to assess vocal, postural, and gestural components of emotional
expression. Further, measures of emotion need to be developed that can be
applied across cultures, cohorts, and species.
o Techniques of computer science, neural networks, and image processing need to
be applied to the task of producing valid and reliable judgments of facial and
other behavioral expressions of emotion.
o Expanded and improved neuroimaging techniques are needed to examine CNS
activity in emotional responding. Further research is needed on the
methodological and conceptual relationships among techniques with different
spatial and temporal resolution.
o Animal models need to be used to their fullest potential to examine social
and biological determinants and consequences of emotion.
o Advanced and ethically-guided human laboratory procedures for inducing
positive and negative emotional states are needed.
INCLUSION OF WOMEN AND MINORITIES IN RESEARCH INVOLVING HUMAN SUBJECTS
It is the policy of the NIH that women and members of minority groups and their
subpopulations must be included in all NIH supported biomedical and behavioral
research projects involving human subjects, unless a clear and compelling
rationale and justification is provided that inclusion is inappropriate with
respect to the health of the subjects or the purpose of the research. This
policy results from the NIH Revitalization Act of 1993 (Section 492B of Public
Law 103-43).
All investigators proposing research involving human subjects should read the
"NIH Guidelines For Inclusion of Women and Minorities as Subjects in Clinical
Research," which have been published in the Federal Register of March 28, 1994
(FR 59 14508-14513) and in the NIH Guide for Grants and Contracts, Vol. 23, No.
11, March 18, 1994 available on the web at the following URL address:
http://grants.nih.gov/grants/guide/notice-files/not94-100.html
INCLUSION OF CHILDREN AS PARTICIPANTS IN RESEARCH INVOLVING HUMAN SUBJECTS
It is the policy of NIH that children (i.e., individuals under the age of 21)
must be included in all human subjects research, conducted or supported by the
NIH, unless there are scientific and ethical reasons not to include them. This
policy applies to all initial (Type 1) applications submitted for receipt dates
after October 1, 1998.
All investigators proposing research involving human subjects should read the
"NIH Policy and Guidelines on the Inclusion of Children as Participants in
Research Involving Human Subjects" that was published in the NIH Guide for
Grants and Contracts, March 6, 1998, and is available at the following URL
address: http://grants.nih.gov/grants/guide/notice-files/not98-024.html
Investigators also may obtain copies of these policies from the program staff
listed under INQUIRIES. Program staff may also provide additional relevant
information concerning the policy.
NOTE FOR APPLICATIONS FOCUSED ON AGING RESEARCH
Applications received in response to this PA may focus on scientific issues
related to aging. In describing the plan to recruit human subjects
investigators may cite a focus on aging as the justification for why children
will be excluded. In this regard, applicants may use Justification 1 from the
policy announcement, the research topic to be studied is irrelevant to children.
SPECIAL NOTE:
Describe any requirements for the inclusion in the application of plans to share
data and indicate that reviewers will assess the adequacy of the proposed plan.
Include this in review criteria section
URLS IN NIH GRANT APPLICATIONS OR APPENDICES
All applications and proposals for NIH funding must be self-contained within
specified page limitations. Unless otherwise specified in an NIH solicitation,
internet addresses (URLs) should not be used to provide information necessary to
the review because reviewers are under no obligation to view the Internet sites.
Reviewers are cautioned that their anonymity may be compromised when they
directly access an Internet site.
APPLICATION PROCEDURES
Applicants are strongly encouraged to contact the program contacts listed under
INQUIRIES with any questions regarding their proposed project and the goals of
this PA.
Applications are to be submitted on the grant application form PHS 398 (rev.
4/98) and will be accepted at the standard application deadlines as indicated
in the application kit. Application kits are available at most institutional
offices of sponsored research and from the Division of Extramural Outreach and
Information Resources, National Institutes of Health, 6701 Rockledge Drive, MSC
7910, Bethesda, MD 20892-7910, telephone (301) 710-0267, Email:
GrantsInfo@nih.gov. Applications are also available on the World Wide Web at:
http://grants.nih.gov/grants/forms.htm.
The modular grant concept establishes specific modules in which direct costs may
be requested as well as a maximum level for requested budgets. Only limited
budgetary information is required under this approach. The just-in-time concept
allows applicants to submit certain information only when there is a possibility
for an award. It is anticipated that these changes will reduce the
administrative burden for the applicants, reviewers and Institute staff. The
research grant application form PHS 398 (rev. 4/98) is to be used in applying
for these grants, with the modifications noted below.
BUDGET INSTRUCTIONS
Modular Grant applications will request direct costs in $25,000 modules, up to a
total direct cost request of $250,000 per year. (Applications that request more
than $250,000 direct costs in any year must follow the traditional PHS 398
application instructions.) The total direct costs must be requested in
accordance with the program guidelines and the modifications made to the
standard PHS 398 application instructions described below:
PHS 398
o FACE PAGE: Items 7a and 7b should be completed, indicating Direct Costs (in
$25,000 increments up to a maximum of $250,000) and Total Costs [Modular Total
Direct plus Facilities and Administrative (F&A) costs] for the initial budget
period Items 8a and 8b should be completed indicating the Direct and Total Costs
for the entire proposed period of support.
o DETAILED BUDGET FOR THE INITIAL BUDGET PERIOD - Do not complete Form Page 4 of
the PHS 398. It is not required and will not be accepted with the application.
o BUDGET FOR THE ENTIRE PROPOSED PERIOD OF SUPPORT - Do not complete the
categorical budget table on Form Page 5 of the PHS 398. It is not required and
will not be accepted with the application.
o NARRATIVE BUDGET JUSTIFICATION - Prepare a Modular Grant Budget Narrative
page. (See http://grants.nih.gov/grants/funding/modular/modular.htm for sample
pages.) At the top of the page, enter the total direct costs requested for each
year. This is not a Form page.
o Under Personnel, List key project personnel, including their names, percent of
effort, and roles on the project. No individual salary information should be
provided. However, the applicant should use the NIH appropriation language
salary cap and the NIH policy for graduate student compensation in developing
the budget request.
o For Consortium/Contractual costs, provide an estimate of total costs (direct
plus facilities and administrative) for each year, each rounded to the nearest
$1,000. List the individuals/organizations with whom consortium or contractual
arrangements have been made, the percent effort of key personnel, and the role
on the project. Indicate whether the collaborating institution is foreign or
domestic. The total cost for a consortium/contractual arrangement is included
in the overall requested modular direct cost amount. Include the Letter of
Intent to establish a consortium.
o Provide an additional narrative budget justification for any variation in the
number of modules requested.
o BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH - The Biographical Sketch provides information used by
reviewers in the assessment of each individual"s qualifications for a specific
role in the proposed project, as well as to evaluate the overall qualifications
of the research team. A biographical sketch is required for all key personnel,
following the instructions below. No more than three pages may be used for each
person. A sample biographical sketch may be viewed at:
http://grants.nih.gov/grants/funding/modular/modular.htm
- Complete the educational block at the top of the form page,
- List position(s) and any honors,
- Provide information, including overall goals and responsibilities, on research
projects ongoing or completed during the last three years.
- List selected peer-reviewed publications, with full citations,
o CHECKLIST - This page should be completed and submitted with the application.
If the F&A rate agreement has been established, indicate the type of agreement
and the date. All appropriate exclusions must be applied in the calculation of
the F&A costs for the initial budget period and all future budget years.
o The applicant should provide the name and phone number of the individual to
contact concerning fiscal and administrative issues if additional information is
necessary following the initial review.
Applications not conforming to these guidelines will be considered unresponsive
to this PA and will be returned without further review.
Applicants planning to submit an investigator-initiated new (type 1), competing
continuation (type 2), competing supplement, or any amended/revised version of
the preceding grant application types requesting $500,000 or more in direct
costs for any year are advised that he or she must contact the Institute program
staff before submitting the application, i.e., as plans for the study are being
developed. Furthermore, the application must obtain agreement from the
Institute staff that the Institute will accept the application for consideration
for award. Finally, the applicant must identify, in a cover letter sent with
the application, the staff member and Institute who agreed to accept assignment
of the application.
This policy requires an applicant to obtain agreement for acceptance of both any
such application and any such subsequent amendment. Refer to the NIH Guide for
Grants and Contracts, March 20, 1998 at
http://grants.nih.gov/grants/guide/notice-files/not98-030.html
The title and number of the program announcement must be typed on line 2 of the
face page of the application form and the YES box must be marked.
Submit a signed, typewritten original of the application, including the
Checklist, and five signed photocopies in one package to:
CENTER FOR SCIENTIFIC REVIEW
NATIONAL INSTITUTES OF HEALTH
6701 ROCKLEDGE DRIVE, ROOM 1040, MSC 7710
BETHESDA, MD 20892-7710
BETHESDA, MD 20817 (for express/courier service)
REVIEW CONSIDERATIONS
Applications will be assigned on the basis of established PHS referral
guidelines. Applications will be evaluated for scientific and technical merit
by an appropriate scientific review group convened in accordance with the
standard NIH peer review procedures. As part of the initial merit review, all
applications will receive a written critique and undergo a process in which only
those applications deemed to have the highest scientific merit, generally the
top half of applications under review, will be discussed, assigned a priority
score, and receive a second level review by the appropriate national advisory
council or board.
Review Criteria
The goals of NIH-supported research are to advance our understanding of
biological systems, improve the control of disease, and enhance health. In the
written comments reviewers will be asked to discuss the following aspects of the
application in order to judge the likelihood that the proposed research will
have a substantial impact on the pursuit of these goals. Each of these criteria
will be addressed and considered in assigning the overall score, weighting them
as appropriate for each application. Note that the application does not need to
be strong in all categories to be judged likely to have major scientific impact
and thus deserve a high priority score. For example, an investigator may
propose to carry out important work that by its nature is not innovative but is
essential to move a field forward.
(1) Significance: Does this study address an important problem? If the aims of
the application are achieved, how will scientific knowledge be advanced? What
will be the effect of these studies on the concepts or methods that drive this
field?
(2) Approach: Are the conceptual framework, design, methods, and analyses
adequately developed, well-integrated, and appropriate to the aims of the
project? Does the applicant acknowledge potential problem areas and consider
alternative tactics?
(3) Innovation: Does the project employ novel concepts, approaches or methods?
Are the aims original and innovative? Does the project challenge existing
paradigms or develop new methodologies or technologies?
(4) Investigator: Is the investigator appropriately trained and well suited to
carry out this work? Is the work proposed appropriate to the experience level
of the principal investigator and other researchers (if any)?
(5) Environment: Does the scientific environment in which the work will be done
contribute to the probability of success? Do the proposed experiments take
advantage of unique features of the scientific environment or employ useful
collaborative arrangements? Is there evidence of institutional support?
In addition to the above criteria, in accordance with NIH policy, all
applications will also be reviewed with respect to the following:
o The adequacy of plans to include both genders, minorities and their
subgroups, and children as appropriate for the scientific goals of the research.
Plans for the recruitment and retention of subjects will also be evaluated.
o The reasonableness of the proposed budget and duration in relation to the
proposed research
o The adequacy of the proposed protection for humans, animals or the
environment, to the extent they may be adversely affected by the project
proposed in the application.
o The adequacy of the proposed plan to share data, if appropriate.
AWARD CRITERIA
Applications will compete for available funds with all other recommended
applications assigned to the Institute. The following will be considered in
making funding decisions: Quality of the proposed project as determined by peer
review, availability of funds, and program priority.
INQUIRIES
Inquiries are encouraged. The opportunity to clarify any issues or questions
from potential applicants is welcome.
Direct inquiries regarding programmatic issues to:
Mary Ellen Oliveri, Ph.D.
Behavioral Science Research Branch
Division of Neuroscience and Basic Behavioral Science
National Institute of Mental Health
6001 Executive Blvd., Room 7220, MSC 9651
Bethesda, MD 20892-9651 (20852 for courier/express service)
Telephone: (301) 443-3942
FAX: (301) 443-9876
Email: moliveri@nih.gov
Jared B. Jobe, Ph.D.
Behavioral and Social Research Program
National Institute on Aging
Gateway Building, Room 533
7201 Wisconsin Avenue
Bethesda, MD 20892
Telephone: (301) 496-3137
FAX: (301) 402-0051
Email: Jared_Jobe@nih.gov
Ellen D. Witt, Ph.D.
Neuroscience and Behavioral Research Branch
National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism
6000 Executive Blvd., Suite 402, MSC 7003
Bethesda, MD 20892-7003
Telephone: (301) 443-6545
FAX: (301) 594-0673
Email: ewitt@willco.niaaa.nih.gov
Wendy Nelson, Ph.D.
Basic Biobehavioral Research Branch
Behavioral Research Program
National Cancer Institute
6130 Executive Blvd., MSC 7326
Executive Plaza North, Room 211
Bethesda, MD 20892-7326
Telephone: 301-435-4590
FAX: 301-435-7547
Email: wn14x@nih.gov
Margaret M. Feerick, Ph.D.
Child Development and Behavior Branch
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
6100 Executive Blvd., Room 4B05, MSC 7510
Bethesda, MD 20892-7510
Telephone: (301) 435-6882
FAX: (301) 480-7773
Email: feerickm@mail.nih.gov
Jaylan S. Turkkan, Ph.D.
Behavioral Sciences Research Branch
National Institute on Drug Abuse
6001 Executive Blvd., Room 4282, MSC 9555
Bethesda, MD 20892-9555
Telephone: (301) 443-1263
FAX: (301) 594-6043
Email: jaylan@nih.gov
Emmeline Edwards, Ph.D.
Systems and Cognitive Neuroscience
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
6001 Executive Blvd., Room 2109
Bethesda, MD 20892-9521
Telephone: (301) 496-9964
FAX: (301) 402-2060
Email: ee48r@nih.gov
Direct inquiries regarding fiscal matters to:
Diana S. Trunnell
Grants Management Branch
National Institute of Mental Health
6001 Executive Boulevard, Room 6115, MSC 9605
Bethesda, MD 20892-9605
Telephone: (301) 443-2805
FAX: (301) 443-6885
Email: Diana_Trunnell@nih.gov
Joseph Ellis
Grants Management Office
National Institute on Aging
Gateway Building, Room 2N-212
7201 Wisconsin Avenue
Bethesda, MD 20892
Telephone: (301) 496-1472
FAX: (301) 402-3672
Email: ellisJ@exmur.nia.nih.gov
Judy Simons
Grants Management Branch
National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism
6000 Executive Blvd., Suite 504, MSC 7003
Bethesda, MD 20892-7003
Telephone: (301) 443-2434
FAX: (301) 443-3891
Email: js182a@nih.gov
Sara Stone
Grants Administration Branch
National Cancer Institute
6120 Executive Blvd., Room 243, MSC 7150
Bethesda, MD 20892-7150
Telephone: (301) 496-7249
FAX: (301) 496-8601
Email: stones@gab.nci.nih.gov
Doug Shawver
Grants Management Branch
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
6100 Executive Boulevard, Room 8A01C, MSC 7510
Bethesda, MD 20892-7510
Telephone: (301) 496-1303
FAX: (301) 402-0915
Email: ds117g@nih.gov
Gary Fleming, J.D., M.A.
Grants Management Branch
National Institute on Drug Abuse
6001 Executive Boulevard, Room 3131, MSC 9541
Bethesda, MD 20892-9541
Telephone: (301) 443-6710
FAX: (301) 594-6849
Email: gf6s@nih.gov
Dawn Richardson
Grants Management Branch
National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke
6001 Executive Blvd., Room 3254
Bethesda, MD 20892-9537
Telephone: (301)496-9231
FAX: (301) 402-0219
Email: da8h@nih.gov
AUTHORITY AND REGULATIONS
This program is described in the Catalog of Federal Domestic Assistance No.
93.242 (NIMH), 93.866 (NIA), 93.273 (NIAAA), 93.399(NCI), 93.865 (NICHD), 93.279
(NIDA), and 93.853 (NINDS). Awards are made under authorization of the Public
Health Service Act, Title IV, Part A (Public Law 78-410, as amended by Public
Law 99-158, 42 USC 241 and 285) and administered under PHS grants policies and
Federal Regulations 42 CFR 52 and 45 CFR Part 74. This program is not subject
to the intergovernmental review requirements of Executive Order 12372 or Health
Systems Agency review.
The PHS strongly encourages all grant and contract recipients to provide a
smoke-free workplace and promote the non-use of all tobacco products. In
addition, Public Law 103-227, the Pro-Children Act of 1994, prohibits smoking in
certain facilities (or in some cases, and portion of a facility) in which
regular or routine education, library, day care, health care or early childhood
development services are provided to children. This is consistent with the PHS
mission to protect and advance the physical and mental health of the American
people.
Weekly TOC for this Announcement
NIH Funding Opportunities and Notices
|
|
|
|
Department of Health and Human Services (HHS)
|
|
|
|
NIH... Turning Discovery Into Health®
|